A much later (early fourteenth century?) satirical image of a medieval schoolroom featuring monkeys!
So when we last left Guibert, he was seven-years-old or thereabouts (he doesn’t specify his age) and struggling to learn the basics of Latin grammar – all those declensions and conjugations back in the days before you had all those handy noun and verb tables to memorise and Kennedy’s Latin Primer, the Cambridge Latin Course or whatever textbook you think is best for beginners (though nowadays very few people start learning as young as Guibert did). Now Guibert seems to have reflected a lot on what his education was like and the difficulties that came with learning and memorising a new language. In the process he came up with some ideas that felt uncannily familiar when I was reading just a few weeks ago – like the theories of cognitive neuroscience we covered as part of our learning and memory sessions in September for the PGCE.
Now I must admit here that I will be engaging in a certain
degree of anachronism in my reading of Guibert, which some medievalist scholars
would find immediately off-putting. Obviously, psychology as an academic discipline
didn’t exist in the early twelfth century, nor would it until more than 750
years after Guibert’s death, and so Guibert would not have used the terms I will
be using to refer to what he was describing. If a time-travelling cognitive
neuroscientist were able to have a conversation with Guibert, it would take a
long time before they could reach some mutual comprehension, since all the
technical language of modern psychology would be completely alien to him. Even
then, this hinges on the whole meta-question of how mentally similar were
medieval people to us. This is an issue that is super-divisive to medievalists,
who basically fall somewhere along a spectrum on this. At one end of the
spectrum, you have those who think medieval people were basically people
exactly like us – they just had swords, horses, heavy-ploughs, parchment and
candles rather than automatic firearms, cars, combine-harvesters, laptops and electrical
lighting. At the other end of the spectrum, you have medievalists who argue
that medieval people were so mentally different from us that they might as well
be Martian visitors – according to them, medieval people were incapable of thinking
rationally like we do except according to their own weird logic, and their
worldview is incomprehensible unless understood completely in its own terms.
Both extremes in my view are unhelpful – the first is essentially the costume
drama version of history. The latter is basically a postmodern repackaging of old
stereotypes about medieval people as violent, uncouth, superstitious,
prejudiced and lacking in individuality. Most medievalists, including myself,
however, sensibly fall somewhere in the middle. And Guibert I feel nicely illustrates
that. All his talk of sin, demons and God’s providence feels very alien to us
and very evocative of the culture of twelfth century France, which was indeed
very different to that of twenty-first century Britain. But strip those layers
of paint away, and one can see a deeply insecure but highly intelligent mind
trying to make sense of his own abnormal life experiences and the rapidly changing
world around him.
But let’s get back to the classroom with Guibert. Guibert
was, as we established in the previous post, in a class all by himself. He was
taught by a private tutor called Solomon who, as we saw last time, had received
his own education quite late in life (how late exactly we don’t know) and was probably
not a very competent teacher – Guibert perhaps exaggerates this somewhat;
indeed, as both Paul Archambault and Jay Rubinstein note, Solomon is deliberately
made out by Guibert to be a negative inversion of the ideal schoolmaster
described by the first century AD Roman educator Quintilian. Guibert makes it
very clear to his readers that his school days were an unhappy time for him.
For starters, Guibert was placed under the complete
authority of his tutor, who essentially controlled his upbringing and daily schedule
from then on. This was not uncommon for that time. Peter Abelard (1079 – 1142)
was given the same kind of power over Heloise when her uncle, Fulbert, hired
him to be her private tutor and, because he already had a crush on her, he
accepted the contract for this very reason. This one of the reasons why a lot
of twenty-first century readers of Abelard and Heloise’s story find it less
romantic and more creepy. What this kind of arrangement meant for Guibert was
that he couldn’t live the same lifestyle as the other children growing up at
his family castle. Guibert recounts that:
I wasn’t even allowed to play the usual games. I couldn’t
go anywhere without his permission, couldn’t eat outside the house, couldn’t
accept a gift from anybody without his consent. I couldn’t do anything “intemperate”,
whether in thought, word, or deed. He seemed to expect me to behave more like a
monk than a cleric. The other boys of my age could come and go as they pleased
and, at times, with no constraints at all. I, on the other hand, was scrupulously
guarded from such behaviour. I would sit in my cleric’s garb and watch the
squads of players like a trained animal. Even on Sundays and saints’ feast days
I had to put up with the constraints of this scholastic system. There was not a
day, not a moment, when I was allowed a holiday. It was study, study, study all
the time. Besides, when he had accepted my tutorship, my master was not allowed
to take on any other student.
Guibert was thus, from the age of seven or possibly even six,
allowed no school holidays, no play-time and no friends. For the time, that was
weird enough, and nowadays child protection agencies would doubtless get involved.
And in Guibert’s estimation, all of this relentless studying was all for
naught, because Solomon was an awful teacher – a stern disciplinarian, yet
completely incompetent when it came to his own subject knowledge (Latin
language and literature). Guibert relates:
Because he worked me so hard everybody who watched us was
convinced that with so much perseverance he would considerably sharpen my
fledgling mind. Alas! This didn’t happen. My master was completely ignorant of
the techniques of composition or metrics. Meanwhile I was deluged everyday with
a hail of blows and whippings. This man was trying to force me to learn what he
couldn’t teach!
Guibert’s brilliant sense of irony really does become
apparent here! Now, if we measure Solomon up to the areas of professional
learning used by my PGCE programme to assess student teachers like myself,
Solomon would be strong (indeed, too strong) on behaviour management, but
incredibly weak on pedagogy, curriculum, assessment and professional behaviours.
Back in the eleventh century there weren’t really any teacher training
programmes, but today Solomon definitely wouldn’t be awarded Qualified Teacher
Status. And his use of punishments, seen as unnecessarily severe by eleventh
century standards, would be regarded today as professional misconduct/ child
abuse. So, by the standards of the time, Solomon was frankly not worth the
money, and by the standards of the present, he would be barred from the
teaching profession.
This being said, Guibert’s view of Solomon isn’t entirely
negative. On the contrary, he says that he taught him “everything pertaining to
modesty, chastity and good manners.” So at least he got something out of his
education, but not what his mother paid for.
Guibert reflects quite deeply on this, and in the process comes
up with some theories of how the education of children should work.
Any person’s nature, let alone a child’s, ends up being
blunted if it has to submit to too much intellectual work. The more a mind is
fired up by extended study, the more the spirit cools as the energies become overexerted.
Energy dissolves into apathy.
How cognitive neuroscientists think memory works
What Guibert is describing here sounds a lot like what neuroscientists
and educational theorists call “cognitive overload.” The theory of cognitive
overload is essentially that new information has to be processed through the
brain’s working memory before it can be committed to the long-term memory. The
working memory can typically only process seven things at a time. Giving too
much information to students at any one stage in the lesson, or getting them to
do too many tasks, leads to cognitive overload as there’s just too much
information for them to process. Therefore, what’s recommended to trainee
teachers like myself is to chunk knowledge and tasks between different stages
of the lesson/ different lessons to make it more manageable and easier to take
in. Unnecessary or distracting information, meanwhile, gets cut out and
scaffolds for complex tasks like prompts, instructions broken down into stages,
templates, tables and other tools are provided so that not too much mental
effort has to be made at any one time. Most people see cognitive load as cutting-edge
educational theory, but Guibert was already thinking about this in the early
twelfth century. Guibert then writes:
If, as Scripture puts it, “there was silence in heaven
for half an hour [Revelation 8:1],” even the gift of contemplation cannot be
sustained unceasingly. It is the same thing for any activity of the mind: it
cannot be maintained without interruption. It is my belief, then, that any mind
concentrating on a specific object should use varying degrees of attention.
Alternately thinking about one thing, then another, we should be able to come
back to the one that our mind is most interested in, as if renewed by the
recreation we have given ourselves. Nature, too, tends to get tired and should
find its remedy in a variety of activities. We must remember that God did not
create a uniform world but allowed us to enjoy time changes – days and nights,
spring and summer, autumn and winter. People who call themselves schoolteachers
should find ways of varying the education of children and young people. Even
students who have the seriousness of old people about them should not be
treated any differently, in my opinion.
In a sense, what Guibert is recommending here is now enshrined
in the modern school day timetable. In mainstream English schools, you will
typically have five to eight lessons a day, each of them typically between 40
and 60 minutes in length. Normally, you will have all these lessons in
different subjects – at most, you will have two in the same subject in the same
day. You will also have an approximately 20-minute morning breaktime and a 40-to-60-minute
lunch period. But many educational theorists see that as not providing enough
variety, rest and focus in itself. Instead, they have argued for things like spaced
learning and interleaving. Spaced learning is when a topic is spread out over a
long period of time – rather than learning it all in one lesson, you instead
spread it out over several lessons or even several weeks while mixing it in
with unrelated stuff. As Guibert might have been hinting at here, it requires
students to immediately commit things to their long term memory rather than try
and hold them in their working memory and then retrieve them as and when
required – its quite good for retrieval practice (being tested on previously
learned content – something I try and do a lot in my lessons) and revision. Interleaving
is when, during a lesson, you take a break from the current content to look at a
slightly different but related topic within the same subject area, before
coming back to the topic you’re currently studying. Both the structure of the
modern school day, interleaving and spaced learning sound very similar to what Guibert
was recommending, and are based on the same logic.
Guibert sums up thus how shambolic the education he received
was, and in doing so imparts some truly timeless wisdom:
While my master was taking it out on me for not knowing
what he himself did not know, he might have been well advised to consider the
harm he had done by squeezing out of my frail little head what he had never put
there in the first place. Lunatics’ words can be barely understood by the sane,
if at all; similarly the utterances of people who are ignorant but pretend to
know something, and who pass on their “knowledge” to others, become even
murkier when they attempt to explain what they are saying. There is nothing
harder than trying to hold forth on something you cannot understand. It is
obscure for the speaker, and even more so for the hearer; it is really as if
both were being turned to stone. I’m saying this, O Lord, not because I want to
stigmatise this man who, all things considered, was a good friend, but in order
to let the readers know, whoever they might be, that we must not be entitled to
teach as truth anything that crosses our minds. Let us not lose other people in
the clouds of our own theories.
Sources cited:
A Monk’s Confession: The Memoirs of Guibert de Nogent,
translated and with an introduction by Pail J Archambault, University of
Pennsylvania Press (1996), pp 16 – 19
Jay Rubinstein, Guibert de Nogent: Portrait of a Medieval
Mind, Routledge (2013), p 13
Education Endowment Foundation, Cognitive Neuroscience in
the Classroom: A Review of the Evidence (2021)